, 1994) The Ucns, NKs, N/OFQ,

and NPS have activity prof

, 1994). The Ucns, NKs, N/OFQ,

and NPS have activity profiles that in part fall into these prototypical categories but also differ from them in being more complex. Here, we will review key findings on each of the individual systems, discuss their similarities and differences, attempt to integrate their interrelationship and the anatomical structures through which they may interact, and identify knowledge gaps that need to be filled. The first member of the CRF/Ucn family to be isolated, CRF, was originally discovered for its crucial role in activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Vale et al., 1981). Subsequently, CRF was shown to also mediate a broad range of coordinated learn more physiological and behavioral stress responses, as well as neuroadaptations that contribute to the development of addiction (Heilig and Koob, 2007; Koob and Zorrilla, 2010; Shalev et al., 2010). With the

discovery of Ucn:s (Ucn1, Ucn2, and Ucn3), it has become clear that the complexity of the CRF/Ucn system is greater than initially appreciated (Lewis et al., 2001; Lovenberg et al., 1995; Potter et al., 1991; Reyes et al., 2001; Vaughan et al., 1995). While the Ucn:s share 20%–45% sequence homology with CRF, physiological functions of CRF/Ucn family peptides are not highly conserved. For example, Ucn2 and Ucn3 do not directly influence Selleckchem SAHA HDAC stress reactivity but instead alter social behaviors in mice, suggesting that mammals have adapted these peptides for regulation of social interactions (Breu et al., 2012; Deussing et al., 2010). Figure 1 presents a schematic of the contribution

of the Ucn system to stress- and addiction-related behaviors. CRF type-1 and CRF type-2 receptors (CRF1R and CRF2R) are both members of the class B/secretin family of heptahelical receptors and are encoded by Crhr1 and Crhr2 genes, respectively. The Crhr2 gene gives rise to at least two alternatively spliced isoforms: CRF2(a), DNA ligase expressed in neurons, and CRF2(b), expressed in peripheral tissues and nonneuronal brain structures ( Bale and Vale, 2004). CRF2(a) and CRF1 receptors share approximately 70% amino acid sequence homology, with a particularly high degree of conservation in regions thought to be the primary site of G protein coupling and signal transduction. Functional specificity of the CRF receptors appears to arise from their distinct cellular expression patterns, anatomical distributions, or both. CRF is largely a CRF1R agonist and displays 18-fold greater affinity for CRF1R than CRF2R (Vaughan et al., 1995). In contrast, Ucn:s are high-affinity agonists for CRF2R, with varying degrees of affinity for CRF1R. Ucn1 binds both receptor subtypes with high affinity, and Ucn1-positive fibers innervate regions expressing both receptors, while Ucn2 and Ucn3 are highly CRF2R selective (Bittencourt et al., 1999; Fekete and Zorrilla, 2007).

g , Roth and Balch, 2011) It seems likely that for each misfoldi

g., Roth and Balch, 2011). It seems likely that for each misfolding-prone protein certain types of neurons are more affected by how that protein disrupts cellular protein networks, and this may contribute to their selective vulnerability to a particular NDD (Figure 1). Indeed, consistent with dominant interference in subsets of neurons, genetic studies in C. elegans have provided evidence that disease-related human proteins such as α-synuclein preferentially form aggregates in certain worm neurons, where they

enhance the vulnerability of the same neurons to misfolding-prone protein species such as constructs with subthreshold polyglutamine stretches ( Brignull et al., 2006 and Lim et al., 2008). Furthermore, mutant misfolding Adriamycin concentration proteins associated with familial forms of NDDs can, at least to some extent, model the same diseases when expressed ubiquitously in evolutionarily distant model organisms such as zebrafish or Drosophila Crizotinib chemical structure (e.g., Lessing and Bonini, 2009, Sheng et al., 2010 and Xia, 2010). These studies are consistent with the notion that disease-associated misfolding proteins each interfere with cellular signaling and proteostasis networks in their own specific manners, thereby affecting preferentially

particular subtypes of neurons whose properties are evolutionarily conserved. Notably, the accumulation of misfolding proteins is often not sufficient to cause disease, and studies of human populations suggest how additional

factors have to combine with the age-related accumulation of misfolding proteins for disease to develop. Thus, the same types of characteristic macroscopic deposits can accumulate in the same neurons or the same brain regions in some but not all aging brains in the absence of major disease manifestations (Jellinger, 2004, Brignull et al., 2006 and Kern and Behl, 2009; but see Sperling et al., 2009 and Hedden et al., 2009). Recent studies have provided intriguing insights into how deposit accumulation may relate to dysfunction in the absence or presence of disease. The studies combined amyloid and functional brain imaging and revealed that aged persons with deposits, but without second noticeable AD, exhibit cognitive deficits involving cortical “default networks,” i.e., cortical areas that are active even when the brain is not engaged and which may be involved in off-line processing. Comparable impairments were detected in patients with mild cognitive deficits, which frequently progress to develop full-blown AD, suggesting that the amyloid deposits may be associated with very early stages of AD (Sperling et al., 2009 and Hedden et al., 2009). Such early stages may not necessarily progress to AD, and the mechanisms underlying disease conversion remain to be determined.

We found that binding of the Slit

C-terminal domain to dy

We found that binding of the Slit

C-terminal domain to dystroglycan requires Ca2+, since addition of EDTA is sufficient to abolish this Slit-dystroglycan interaction (Figure 6E). Moreover, a version of the Slit2 C-terminal domain in which two basic residues adjacent to the Ca2+ binding site are mutated to alanine (K1177A, R1179A, referred to here as Slit2 C-term AVA) is incapable of binding to Fc-dystroglycan (Figure 6F). Thus, the Slit2 LG domain mediates its association with dystroglycan and, similar to other LG modules, the Slit2 LG domain requires a Ca2+ binding site surrounded by a basic patch for this interaction. Our findings that Slit can bind directly to dystroglycan in vitro raise the intriguing possibility that dystroglycan Roxadustat mouse present in the floor plate and basement membrane serves as a scaffold for the proper localization of Slit in vivo. Consistent with this idea, dystroglycan and slit are required for proper cardiac tube formation in Drosophila, and Slit protein appears to be mislocalized in dystroglycan mutant cardioblasts

( Medioni et al., 2008). We first verified that the expression patterns of Slit1 and Slit2 mRNA are indistinguishable in control and B3gnt1 mutants ( Figure S7A), demonstrating that dystroglycan is not required for floor plate development or expression of these axonal guidance cues. To test whether dystroglycan I-BET151 regulates Slit localization, an AP-section binding assay was employed to visualize the location of endogenous C-terminal Slit binding sites in vivo. Incubation of transverse spinal cord sections from E11 control embryos with the AP-Slit C-term ligand showed robust binding to the basement membrane surrounding the spinal cord and the floor plate ( Figure 7A), regions that are enriched for dystroglycan protein expression ( Figures 3C and 3D). Importantly, binding of AP-Slit C-term is absent in B3gnt1LacZ/M115T mutants, demonstrating that glycosylation of dystroglycan is essential for Slit C-terminal domain binding in vivo. Since Slit binds directly

to glycosylated dystroglycan via its C-terminal LG domain, Dichloromethane dehalogenase we hypothesized that dystroglycan present in both the floor plate and basement membrane are required for organization of endogenous Slit proteins within these locations. Therefore, we developed a method to assess the sites of Slit protein localization in tissue sections to ask whether loss of glycosylated dystroglycan in the B3gnt1 mutants alters the distribution of endogenous Slit protein in vivo. The lack of antibodies suitable for mammalian Slit immunolocalization necessitated the development of an alternate approach. Therefore, we modified the AP-ligand section binding assay by using an AP-Robo ectodomain fusion protein that is capable of binding to Slit protein on tissue sections ( Jaworski and Tessier-Lavigne, 2012).

Such discordance, for the most part not yet understood in detail,

Such discordance, for the most part not yet understood in detail, is grounded in complex interactions of genes with stochastic SKI-606 cost and environmental factors

that influence brain development, maturation, and function. That said, genomes carry enormous biological influence: the remarkable similarities of basic brain structure and function within species are testimony to the central significance of the genetic blueprint. A recent demonstration that human pluripotent stem cells in vitro (extremely distant from a natural developmental environment) can give rise to cerebral organoids with discrete recognizable brain structures and significant features of a cerebral cortex (Lancaster et al., 2013) serves as a remarkable reminder

of the information contained in genomes—even if the resulting organoids are only pale simulacra of a human brain. Genetic information is particularly important to neurobiologists studying brain disease because the human brain is, both for Selleck Vorinostat ethical and practical reasons, generally inviolable. Scientists studying the biology of cancer or immunologic diseases, for example, can have direct access to diseased tissues obtained from surgical specimens or blood. The resulting cells can be examined for somatic mutations, epigenetic marks, patterns of gene expression, and other molecular indicia. In contrast, for the most part, the human brain can only be examined indirectly in life. Thus, when disorders of the CNS have a significant hereditary component of risk, the ability to obtain molecular clues from genetic analysis may create the most effective current opportunities for scientific investigation. The utility of genetic insights is particularly salient in brain

disorders that affect evolutionarily recent brain circuits and regions or that for other reasons have been difficult to model in animals. These include common else psychiatric disorders such as autism, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depression as well as late-onset versions of neurodegenerative disorders such as Parkinson’s disease and Alzheimer’s disease. In the case of the psychiatric disorders, the relative lack of neuropathology that can be analyzed in postmortem tissue makes genetic information even more valuable as a source of molecular clues to pathogenesis. Psychiatric disorders have long been recognized to cluster in families even though they do not segregate in simple, Mendelian fashion. Twin and adoption studies demonstrated that familiality resulted from heredity, thus suggesting that information about the molecular basis of these serious and disabling disorders is hidden in DNA sequence variation.

, 2011) Synaptic depression also shows temporal asymmetry simila

, 2011). Synaptic depression also shows temporal asymmetry similar to that observed here (Hosoya et al., 2005, Dobrunz et al., 1997 and Chung et al., 2002). Gain control is primarily useful for adapting the limited dynamic range of a neuron to the statistics of the stimulus. When spectrotemporal contrast is low, firing rates are sensitive to smaller changes within their spectral “region of interest” than under higher-contrast conditions. Thus, the representation of stimulus space is effectively expanded under low-contrast stimulation and compressed under high-contrast stimulation. Decitabine cost Consequently, gain control should improve the ability of individuals

to detect small changes in low-contrast sounds. Indeed, a related phenomenon has been demonstrated in the adaptation to reverberation, whereby listeners are better able to discriminate

(low-contrast) reverberant words when embedded within a reverberant context sentence than within a (high-contrast) anechoic context (Watkins, 2005), an effect that is also frequency-band specific (Watkins and Makin, 2007). Perceptual adaptation is not, however, complete, as a general increase in the spectrotemporal contrast of speech leads to selleck screening library demonstrable gains in intelligibility (Steeneken and Houtgast, 1980, van Veen and Houtgast, 1985 and Miller et al., 1999). Our data predict that perceptual adaptation to stimulus contrast should be observable with nonspeech stimuli as well. Neurons in the visual system are subject to contrast gain control, which is thought to be desirable for efficient coding of natural images (Schwartz and Simoncelli, 2001). Since the contrast of natural images is correlated across space and time, normalization by stimulus contrast reduces the redundancy of aminophylline the neural code (Barlow, 1961 and Vinje and Gallant, 2002). The contrast of a complex visual stimulus can be defined as σI/μI, which is strongly related to the two contrast measures that we have shown to determine auditory gain control (σL, Figure 5A; σP/μP, Figure S4C).

Auditory gain control may therefore have a similar redundancy-reducing effect. Although the ensemble (i.e., long time scale) distributions of natural sounds have been explored ( Attias and Schreiner, 1997, Escabí et al., 2003 and Singh and Theunissen, 2003), a deeper understanding of the relationship between contrast gain control and the statistics of natural sounds will require a characterization of natural sound level distributions at the temporal scales over which gain control operates. We show that when stimulus level statistics are not uniform across the spectrum, gain control is also unevenly applied to neurons, depending on their frequency tuning. A spectrally limited band of high contrast has the greatest compressive effect on neurons if their tuning curves overlap this band.

, 2009) Then, despite the results observed we suggested that mor

, 2009). Then, despite the results observed we suggested that morphology is not efficient as the PCR in the discrimination of Eimeria species. The analysis of the lesion score shown to be the less effective method for the diagnosis of Eimeria species in the study conditions. Navitoclax cell line First, before the advent of anticoccidial drugs clinical coccidiosis are quite scarce in field operations and clear pathological lesions are

hard to found. Second, different species parasitize the same or very close regions along the intestinal tract of birds could be overlapped with another. Also, when observing pathological macroscopic injuries changes are possible according to the life stage in which animals are evaluated or due to the use of anticoccidial drugs into the diet ( Prado, 2005). Later, even the characteristics of lesions may help in diagnosis, the differences are subtle and require

technical training and much experience to provide reliable results. The implementation of PCR as a routine diagnostic technique in poultry flocks could enhance the monitoring of fluctuations in Eimeria populations, helping in the adoption of specific measures against the parasite without the need to plus unnecessary work, thereby reducing production costs. This research was supported by the Foundation for Research on the state of Bahia (FAPESB) for financial support under DRC No. 0029/2007, Case No. 00 657 1431 1000, National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq) and University State of Santa Cruz (UESC); why Properties poultry collaborators; click here Biovet Laboratory in the person of Dr. Sandra Fernandez for yielding pure samples and isolated DNA to construct the positive control. “
“One of the major constraints of the animal production in the tropical regions is the presence of parasites. Losses in Brazil due to gastrointestinal nematodes are estimated to

be about 68 million dollars a year (Honer and Bianchin, 1987 and Soutello et al., 2002). In Brazil, Haemonchus spp. and Cooperia spp. are the most prevalent nematodes ( Bianchin et al., 2007 and Oliveira et al., 2009). Brazilian Nellore is a beef breed generally considered resistant to many ectoparasites, but it is not clear if this is true in the specific case of gastrointestinal parasites (Holgado and Cruz, 1994 and Oliveira et al., 2009). Although the mechanisms involved in host defense are better understood in Bos taurus ( Sonstegard and Gasbarre, 2001), less information is available for Bos indicus ( Bricarello et al., 2007, Bricarello et al., 2008 and Zaros et al., 2010). Genetic mechanisms underlying the variation of resistance can be related to the development of different profiles of Th1/Th2 cytokines (Meeusen et al., 2005 and Huse et al., 2006).

, 2011) The cellular mechanisms underlying learning-dependent ch

, 2011). The cellular mechanisms underlying learning-dependent changes in white matter microstructure remain to be established, Everolimus supplier and so do the links between these changes and measures of functional plasticity. It has been proposed that changes in white matter properties, indexed by FA, could affect the velocity and synchronicity of impulse conduction between distant cortical regions and thus contribute to the optimization of information flow required for skill acquisition (see Fields, 2008 and Fields, 2011), a hypothesis that requires specific testing. Altogether, demonstrations of learning-induced gray and white matter plasticity in humans represent an exciting development in systems neuroscience. Yet the

contribution of this line of research to our understanding of motor skill learning is still limited. The biological mechanisms that underlie these forms of plasticity remain to be elucidated, and its time scales need to be more clearly established.

Additionally, strict comparative evaluation of structural and functional plasticity associated with motor skill learning Selleck Docetaxel is difficult at this point, given the different experimental paradigms used in the literature. This issue should be overcome in future investigations by evaluating both forms of plasticity in longitudinal studies in the same subjects (Thomas et al., 2009). Progression from fast to slow motor skill learning is thought to rely on appropriate consolidation

(Doyon and Benali, 2005, Muellbacher et al., 2002 and Robertson et al., 2004a), defined as the progressive stabilization of a recently acquired memory (Dudai, 2004). Through consolidation, new memories are transformed from their initial fragile states into more robust and stabile forms (Robertson et al., 2004a). In relation to motor skill learning, the term consolidation has been used in the literature to describe two different, but not mutually exclusive, phenomena: the offline behavioral skill improvements that occur after the end of a practice session (Robertson et al., 2004a) and the reduction in fragility of a motor memory trace that follows about encoding (Robertson, 2009 and Robertson et al., 2004a). In humans, offline skill improvements may be affected by sleep (e.g., Diekelmann and Born, 2010, Fischer et al., 2002 and Korman et al., 2003). Sleep-dependent motor memory consolidation, which correlates with the amount of stage II nonrapid eye movement sleep (Walker et al., 2002), has been mostly demonstrated for explicit motor sequence learning (Fischer et al., 2005, Korman et al., 2003 and Walker et al., 2002; but see Brawn et al., 2010 and Rickard et al., 2008). Other forms of procedural motor learning are not necessarily sleep dependent (Debas et al., 2010, Doyon et al., 2009b and Song et al., 2007). Notably, sleep does not benefit implicit forms of sequence learning (Robertson et al., 2004b and Song et al., 2007).

1 pA, 12 1 Hz ± 0 8 Hz, n = 7; RU486+CORT: 32 4 ± 4 9 pA, 11 3 ±

1 pA, 12.1 Hz ± 0.8 Hz, n = 7; RU486+CORT: 32.4 ± 4.9 pA, 11.3 ± 0.98 Hz, n = 9, p > 0.05) but not the MR antagonist RU28318 (10 μM, RU28318: 33.3 pA ± 4.7 pA,

11.8 Hz ± 1.3 Hz, n = 7; RU28318+CORT: 22.9 pA ± 1.4 pA, 7.4 Hz ± 1.4 Hz, n = 9, p < 0.05), suggesting that GR mediates the effect of chronic CORT treatment. To test whether the CORT-induced reduction of mEPSC was due to the decreased number of AMPARs at synapses, we performed immunocytochemical experiments to measure the cluster density (# clusters/50 μm dendrite) of total GluR1 and synaptic GluR1 (colocalized with the synaptic marker PSD-95) in PFC cultures. As shown in Figures 4E and 4F, CORT treatment (100 nM, 7 day) significantly reduced total GluR1 cluster density INCB024360 cost (control: 26.6 ± 3.1, n = 14; CORT: 15.6 ± Apoptosis Compound Library price 1.3, n = 12, p < 0.01) and synaptic GluR1 cluster density (control: 14.0 ± 1.0, n = 11; CORT: 7.8 ± 0.7, n = 12, p < 0.01). Taken together, these results suggest that, similar to in vivo repeated stress, prolonged in vitro CORT treatment also reduces AMPAR expression and function through GR activation. Since the total level of NR1 and GluR1 was reduced in repeatedly stressed animals, we examined whether it could be due to the decreased synthesis or increased degradation of glutamate receptors. As shown in Figure S4, repeated stress

did not significantly alter the mRNA level of AMPAR and NMDAR subunits, suggesting that protein synthesis is intact. PDK4 Thus, the reducing effect of repeated stress on NR1 and GluR1 expression may be due to the increased ubiquitin/proteasome-dependent

protein degradation. Consistent with this, the level of ubiquitinated GluR1 and NR1 was significantly increased in animals exposed to repeated restraint stress (Figures 5A and 5B, Ub-GluR1: 121.6% ± 28.3% increase, Ub-NR1: 135.9% ± 35.6% increase, n = 6 pairs, p < 0.01), which was abolished by RU486 injection (n = 3). The level of ubiquitinated GluR2, NR2A, or NR2B subunits remained unchanged (n = 4 pairs, Figure 5C). Repeated stress also failed to alter the ubiquitination of SAP97 (a GluR1 binding protein) and PSD-95 (an NR1 binding protein, n = 3 pairs, Figure 5C). These results provide direct evidence showing that prolonged GR activation selectively increases ubiquitin conjugation of GluR1 and NR1 subunits in PFC and thus enhances the susceptibility of these proteins to proteasome-mediated degradation. To further test the role of glutamate receptor degradation in chronic stress-induced reduction of synaptic transmission, we injected the proteasome inhibitor MG132 into PFC via an implanted cannula (0.5 μg each side; 21 pmol/g body weight, daily at 1 hr before stress). As shown in Figures 6A and 6B, the effects of repeated restraint stress on glutamatergic transmission were significantly different in saline- versus MG132-injected animals (AMPA: p < 0.

The process may result from platelet activation or an altered end

The process may result from platelet activation or an altered endothelial or coagulopathic state (Cavestro et al., 2011 and Pezzini et al., 2007). While thrombosis and migraine may be comorbid, the usually microthrombotic events contribute to microinfarcts in this population and also major strokes, usually occurring in the posterior circulation. Increased responses to sensory stimuli in migraineurs are observed interictally and include pain (allodynia), phonophobia, photophobia, and osmophobia. All of these changes are as a consequence of maladaptation with Selleck OSI 906 the disease. With repeated attacks there is evidence of central sensitization of sensory systems. Acute allodynia (pain to a normally

nonnoxious stimulus) is present in over 50% of patients; interictal allodynia is present in over a quarter of patients, greater still in patients who have aura (Lovati et al., 2008). The lowering of the pain threshold with repeated attacks may then allow for further attacks that contribute http://www.selleckchem.com/products/Fulvestrant.html to chronification that is mediated in part by medication overuse (Zappaterra et al., 2011). During the interictal period, episodic and chronic migraineurs are more sensitive to thermal stimulation than nonmigraine controls (Schwedt et al., 2011). In children with migraine, quantitative sensory testing to tonic heat applied to the trigeminal area shows increased sensitivity (Zohsel et al., 2006). Thus, alteration in sensory processing reflects changes

in brain systems that are a consequence of migraine load. Some have suggested that cutaneous allodynia is associated with migraine progression (Bonavita and De Simone, 2010). Other systems also show central sensitization even during the interictal period: phonophobia, osmophobia (Sjöstrand et al., 2010), and photophobia (Purdy, 2011). Abnormal brain activity is present

in studies of allodynia (Burstein to et al., 2010), olfactory hypersensitivity (Demarquay et al., 2008), and photophobia (Denuelle et al., 2011), all showing increased excitability. Such changes point to significant functional rewiring of the brain. Perhaps disease progression (increased frequency of attacks), transformation, or chronification (transformation from episodic to chronic migraine) of the brain state from low-frequency episodic migraine to high-frequency episodic migraine and then to chronic migraine (Bigal and Lipton, 2011) is the sine-qua-non of a measure of allostatic load in this clinical condition. About 6% of migraineurs progress to high-frequency episodic headaches, characterized by 105–179 headache days/year (Bigal and Lipton, 2008). Three percent of individuals in the general population with infrequent episodic headache progress to chronic daily headache (CDH) each year, and approximately 2.5% of patients with episodic migraine develop new-onset chronic migraine (Manack et al., 2011). Given the approximately 8 per 1000 of the population (Lyngberg et al., 2005), or a one-year prevalence of episodic migraine in the US of nearly 12% (Lipton et al.

Genetic deletion of S6K1 in Fmr1 KO mice was successful in correc

Genetic deletion of S6K1 in Fmr1 KO mice was successful in correcting numerous behavioral abnormalities, including social interaction, novel object recognition, and behavioral flexibility ( Figure 6). Our analyses also revealed that S6K1 KO

mice themselves display impaired novel object recognition ( Figure 6B) and abnormal social behavior ( Figure 6C). In contrast, S6K1 KO mice were adept at reversal learning in the Y-maze ( Figures 6D and 6E). In addition, S6K1 KO mice were hypoactive in the open field arena ( Figures S3A and S3B) as reported earlier ( Antion et al., 2008b) but were impaired in rotarod performance ( Figure 6A). We chose to limit our biochemical, electrophysiological, CH5424802 order and morphological studies to the hippocampus due to the extensive data available in this brain area for Fmr1 KO mice. However, many of the behavioral tests we conducted have well-established cortical-, striatal-, and amygdala-dependent selleck products components. We observed no rescue of FXS-associated hyperactivity and marble-burying features in dKO mice, suggesting that there is limited impact of deleting S6K1 on altered corticostriatal circuitry in FXS model mice. However, impairments in novel social and object recognition were rectified, suggesting that the S6K1 removal may results in region-specific correction of cortical impairments in FXS mice. It will

be important to examine molecular and synaptic phenotypes

in other brain regions to obtain a more holistic idea of how the lack of S6K1 wields a corrective influence on the FXS brain. Though FXS is largely considered a disorder of the nervous system, FMRP expression is widespread during development, with postnatal expression limited to neurons and testes (Hinds et al., 1993). This suggests a possible role for nonneuronal FMRP in peripheral phenotypes, the effects of which are felt even of after the actual protein expression has abated. This may be the reason why strategies based on neuronal mediators do not rescue peripheral symptoms such as macro-orchidism and connective tissue abnormalities entirely (Dölen et al., 2007; Michalon et al., 2012). Thus, the correction of macro-orchidism in the dKO mice was likely because S6K1 was constitutively and globally reduced across tissues and organ systems in FXS mice. It remains to be determined whether connective tissue defects in the Fmr1 KO mice also are rescued by deletion of S6K1. We propose that the translational control of specific mRNAs in neurons is dynamically regulated by the opposing actions of FMRP and S6K1 (Figure 8). In WT mice, signaling downstream of cell surface receptors activate mTORC1 and/or ERK that results in activation of S6K1, which promotes protein synthesis via phosphorylation of multiple downstream effectors (Figure 8A).