If so, then this inhibitory modulation might compromise the conso

If so, then this inhibitory modulation might compromise the consolidation of the suppressed memory by, for example, disrupting the replay of its hippocampal representation (Karlsson and Frank, 2009; Carr et al., 2011). As a corollary, inhibition would cause forgetting of the suppressed memory, and individuals who are more effective at inhibiting retrieval would exhibit a

greater degree of forgetting. The direct suppression mechanism shown here may elucidate the causes of mnemonic disorders such as psychogenic amnesia (Tramoni et al., 2009; Kikuchi et al., 2010) but also may help to understand how people cope with intrusive memories in the aftermath of traumatic events (Shin et al., 1999; Lyoo et al., 2011). On one hand, Kikuchi et al. (2010) scanned two neurologically normal patients who could remember new experiences despite exhibiting dense psychogenic retrograde amnesia. When AZD9291 in vitro these patients viewed photographs of faces of acquaintances drawn from the period for which they were amnesic (faces that they did not recognize), Kikuchi

et al. observed greater DLPFC and ventrolateral PFC activation as well as reduced hippocampal activation. This pattern emerged even in comparison with activation for novel faces. Thus, a hyperactivity of the DLPFC-hippocampal circuit observed here might contribute to severe memory disruptions. On the other hand, inhibitory processes supported by DLPFC may help in coping with traumatic experiences. A recent longitudinal study examined the structural brain changes in survivors of a CHIR-99021 concentration subway disaster, and the relation of those changes with the recovery from posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Lyoo et al., 2011). Survivors who exhibited the greatest DLPFC cortical thickness 1 year after the disaster also showed the largest reductions in PTSD symptoms. Moreover, over the course of 3 years, DLPFC

volume normalized to the level of controls with the degree of recovery. Thus, processes supported by this region may foster the control of negative emotions (Ochsner and Gross, 2005) but may also be involved in coping with intrusive memories. Consistent with this idea, PTSD patients exhibit reduced DLPFC recruitment when presented with reminders of traumatic experiences (Shin et al., 1999), and our results show that less DLPFC activation can be linked to less forgetting of CYTH4 reminded memories (see also Anderson et al., 2004; Depue et al., 2007). In contrast, for the thought substitution group, HC activation did not differ reliably between the suppress and recall conditions, and this reduced modulation differed from the modulation observed for the direct suppression group. Given that recalling a memory (whether the original or a substitute) probably always requires engagement of the hippocampus, this dissociation further supports the proposal that the selective HC disengagement during direct suppression reflects a systemic disruption of retrieval.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

*

You may use these HTML tags and attributes: <a href="" title=""> <abbr title=""> <acronym title=""> <b> <blockquote cite=""> <cite> <code> <del datetime=""> <em> <i> <q cite=""> <strike> <strong>